Thư viện tri thức trực tuyến
Kho tài liệu với 50,000+ tài liệu học thuật
© 2023 Siêu thị PDF - Kho tài liệu học thuật hàng đầu Việt Nam

Tài liệu Constituent Structure - Part 4 pptx
Nội dung xem thử
Mô tả chi tiết
Moving from left to right on the page, we combine two elements
creating a new object, then we combine2 this new structure with the
next word to the right, and so on.
(4) (a) Nemo ate
(b) (Nemo ate) Dory’s
(c) ((Nemo ate) Dory’s) seaweed
(d) (((Nemo ate) Dory’s) seaweed)
Let us call this the structured-concatenation hypothesis. This approach does not suVer from the problem of (2), in that any subpart
of (4d) is not identical to a subpart of any of the strings in (3). For
example, the Wrst concatenation in (4) is not identical to the Wrst
concatenation of (3b):
(5) (Nemo ate) 6¼ (Dory ate)
This is what we want, since we do not want sentence (2) to mean the
same thing as (3b). Nevertheless, the structured concatenation hypothesis suVers in a diVerent, important, way. If you look closely at the
brackets in (4d), you will note that Dory’s is structurally closer to ate
than it is to seaweed. We can capture this more precisely by counting
the number of parentheses enclosing each item. If we were to number
matching opening and closing parens, we get the annotated structure
in (6):
(6) (1(2(3Nemo ate)3 Dory’s)2 seaweed)1
You will note that all of {Nemo, ate, Dory’s} are enclosed in the (2)2
parentheses. Seaweed is excluded from this set. In this set-theoretic
sense then, Dory’s is closer to ate than it is to seaweed. However, this
Xies in the face of native-English-speaker intuitions about what words
go together with one another. On an intuitive level, we know that
Dory’s has a closer semantic relationship to seaweed than it does to ate.
You might think we could get around this problem by reversing
the order of concatenation and starting at the right. Indeed, for the
example sentence (1), this gives us a structure corresponding to the
intuition about the closeness of Dory and seaweed:
2 I use the symbol here roughly in the sense in which it is used in Head Driven Phrase
Structure Grammar (HPSG): as a list-addition operator which is not commutative:
a b 6¼ b a. Though technically speaking operates over set-theoretic objects,
I abstract away from this here. Also, the brackets in (4) are not actually part of the representation, I put them in this diagram as a heuristic for the reader. The symbol is meant here as
the rough equivalent of ^ in Chomsky’s Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory (1975).
10 preliminaries