Thư viện tri thức trực tuyến
Kho tài liệu với 50,000+ tài liệu học thuật
© 2023 Siêu thị PDF - Kho tài liệu học thuật hàng đầu Việt Nam

Tài liệu Facial Expressions in Hollywood''''s Portrayal of Emotion ppt
Nội dung xem thử
Mô tả chi tiết
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1997, Vol. 72, No. I, 164-176
Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
OO22-35I4/97/J3.OO
Facial Expressions in Hollywood's Portrayal of Emotion
James M. Carroll and James A. Russell
University of British Columbia
Much theory and research on emotion are based on the facial expressions of amateurs asked to pose
for still photographs. The theory of facial affect programs (FAPs; P. Ekman, 1972) was proposed
to account for the resulting expressions, most of which are patterns consisting of distinguishable
parts. In the present study, 4 Hollywood films noted for fine acting and realism were examined for
the facial expressions that accompany a basic emotion. In keeping with the theory of FAPs, professional actors judged as happy were found smiling in 97% (Duchenne smiling in 74%) of cases. In
contrast, actors judged as surprised, afraid, angry, disgusted, or sad rarely showed the predicted
pattern (found in 0 to 31% of cases). Typically, they used one or two parts from the full pattern. If
these films represent real life, these findings favor a theory that assumes separable parts (e.g.,
components theory) over the older theory of FAPs.
Lazarus (1980) wrote, * The conduct of our affairs is heavily
determined by how we interpret the thoughts and feelings of
others. .. . By watching the faces of others in action, we can
to some extent know how they feel. . . . Even if the expression is modified or disguised, there will be telltale signs" (pp.
vii—viii).
Specific facial patterns have been theorized to be biologically
hardwired signals that allow one person to know the emotion
of another. Izard (1971), Ekman and Friesen (1975, 1976), and
Matsumoto and Ekman (1988) have published now familiar
portraits of the facial expressions they hypothesize occur for
happiness, surprise, fear, anger, disgust, and sadness (the socalled basic emotions). An example of the kind of pattern hypothesized is shown in Figure 1. Shown such expressions, observers from New York to New Guinea, including members of
a nearly Stone Age culture, were claimed to recognize the emotions expressed (Ekman & Friesen, 1971; Ekman, Sorenson, &
Friesen, 1969; Izard, 1971). According to Ekman (1980), it
therefore follows that "when someone feels an emotion and is
not trying to disguise it, his or her face appears the same no
matter who that person is or where he or she comes from"
(p. 7).
But does this inference follow? Do the recognition studies
James M. Carroll and James A. Russell, Department of Psychology,
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
This study was funded by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.
We thank Deborah von Lipinski and Lorraine Russell for providing
the photographs used in this article; Anuradha Chawla, Christine Lilly,
and Bruce McMurtry for their help in gathering and analyzing the data
reported; and Andrea Bull and Lisa Ferguson for their helpful editorial
comments. We thank Pierre Gosselin for providing us with extensive
reanalyses of his data and for insightful comments on a draft of this
article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to James
A. Russell, Department of Psychology, 2136 West Mall, University of
British Columbia, Vancouver British Columbia, Canada V6T 1Z4. Electronic mail may be sent via the Internet to [email protected].
(even assuming they are valid) tell us what facial expressions
actually occur in New "Vbrk, in New Guinea, or in other scenes
from everyday life? Most of the recognition studies were conducted with highly artificial materials. Often the facial stimuli
were still photographs of amateurs asked to portray single emotions for the camera. In these experiments, the process whereby
one person expresses emotion to another was guided by the
experimenter to a degree that raises questions of generalizability
to the nonexperimental world. To be sure, artificial procedures
and materials have their place. Nevertheless, they do not necessarily tell us what occurs in ordinary life.
A more ecological perspective on facial expressions raises
the following kinds of unanswered questions:
1. What is the natural response of observers to the facial
expressions of others? How often do they interpret facial expressions in terms of single, basic emotions? What other interpretations of facial expressions are made? In everyday circumstances,
might the woman of Figure 1 be thought frustrated, overwrought, or histrionic rather than simply angry?
2. What facial behaviors actually occur in everyday life, in
what intensities, durations, configurations, and frequencies? For
example, how often do the specific facial configurations portrayed by Izard, Ekman, and their colleagues actually occur?
Their research and theorizing are largely limited to a small
number of facial configurations relative to all possible combinations of muscle contractions of which the face is capable. Which
configurations actually occur under various emotion-eliciting
circumstances? When one person does attribute a single basic
emotion to another, what facial information, if any, was that
attribution typically based on? For example, in all the occasions
in which Debbie is seen by her friends as angry, how often does
her face look like that shown in Figure 1?
In this article, we begin to explore the second set of questions.
We offer evidence that everyday life might be rather different
from what is implied by Izard's (1971) and Ekman's (1972,
1975) accounts. When observers attribute happiness to Debbie,
she is very likely smiling. On the other hand, when observers
attribute anger, surprise, fear, disgust, or sadness to her, she is
164