Thư viện tri thức trực tuyến
Kho tài liệu với 50,000+ tài liệu học thuật
© 2023 Siêu thị PDF - Kho tài liệu học thuật hàng đầu Việt Nam

Problem Solving and Communicative Action
Nội dung xem thử
Mô tả chi tiết
Journal of Communication ISSN 0021-9916
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Problem Solving and Communicative Action:
A Situational Theory of Problem Solving
Jeong-Nam Kim1 & James E. Grunig2
1 Department of Communication, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907-2098, USA
2 Department of Communication, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742-7635, USA
This article introduces the situational theory of problem solving (theory of problem
solving) as an extended and generalized version of the situational theory of publics (theory
of publics). The theory of problem solving introduces a new concept, communicative
action in problem solving, as its dependent variable. To explain communicative action,
the theory of problem solving refines the independent variables of the theory of publics:
problem recognition, constraint recognition, involvement recognition, and referent criterion.
Finally, it introduces a new variable: situational motivation in problem solving. The
new motivational variable mediates the effects of antecedent perceptual variables on
communicative behavior. We report the supporting evidence for theoretical propositions of
the new theory based on structural equation modeling (SEM) testing using survey data.
doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010.01529.x
‘‘All life is problem solving’’ (Popper, 1999).
The situational theory of publics (theory of publics) has explained when and why
individuals become active in communication behaviors such as information seeking
(Grunig, 2003). Grunig (1966, 1989) developed the situational theory of publics as a
theory of individual communication behavior and decision making. The theory later
moved to a collective level of analysis as the concepts in the individual-level theory
were used to explain and identify who are publics of organizations, as that term is
used in public relations (Grunig, 1997).
The situational theory of publics refined, improved, and formalized two classic
theories of publics and public opinion, those of Dewey (1927) and Blumer (1966).
According to Dewey and Blumer, publics are critical components of the democratic
process that recognize problems that affect them and organize and act similarly to
resolve those problems. Because the situational theory defines and identifies publics
(Grunig, 2003), it has advanced our understanding of the opinions of publics and the
social processes created by their behaviors.
Corresponding author: Jeong-Nam Kim; e-mail: [email protected]
120 Journal of Communication 61 (2011) 120–149 © 2011 International Communication Association
J.-N. Kim & J. E. Grunig Situational Theory of Problem Solving
The theory of publics has three variables that explain and predict communication
behavior (problem recognition, level of involvement, and constraint recognition) and
two dependent variables that describe active and passive communication behavior in
acquiring information (information seeking and attending).1 A person who perceives
a problem, a connection to it, and few obstacles to doing something about it is likely to
seek and attend to information about the problem (Figure 1). Because the situational
Communicative
Action in
Problem
Solving
H1: +
H2: - H5: + Situational
Motivation
in Problem
Solving
H3: +
H4: +
Problem
Recognition
Constraint
Recognition
Referent
Criterion
Involvement
Recognition
Information
Forefending
Information
Attending
Information
Seeking
Information
Sharing
Information
Forwarding
Information
Permitting
+
+
+ +
+ +
Communicative Behavior
in Problem Solving
Situational
Motivation
in Problem
Solving
Situational Antecedents:
Perceptual &
Cognitive Frame
in Problem Solving
Information Selection Information Transmission Information Acquisition
+
-
+
Problem
Recognition
Constraint
Recognition
Level of
Involvement
Information
Processing
Information
Seeking
Information Acquisition
+
+
-
Situational Theory of Publics
Internal Inquiring Stage
(Knowledge Activation)
External Inquiring Stage
(Knowledge Action)
Individual Effectuating Stage
(Individual Solution Application )
Collective Effectuating Stage
(Collective Solution Application )
Situational Motivation in Problem Solving
= f(PR, IR, CR | RC)
Information
Selection
Information
Transmission
Information
Acquisition
Situational Theory of Publics
Situational Theory of Problem Solving
Situational Communicant Activeness in Problem Solving
Problem
Recognition
Active Public Activist Public
IS Top IT Top
IA Top
Situational Theory of Problem Solving
Figure 1 Situational theory of publics and situational theory of problem solving.
Journal of Communication 61 (2011) 120–149 © 2011 International Communication Association 121
Situational Theory of Problem Solving J.-N. Kim & J. E. Grunig
theory has the power to explain and predict who is most likely to communicate
actively on social or individual problems, it has been used heavily by both public
relations theorists and practitioners (Aldoory & Sha, 2006).
The most important lesson from the situational theory is that information
consumption becomes systematic when people find that information matches their
subjective life problems (Grunig, 1997). People selectively invest their communicative
and cognitive resources in a problem only when they perceive the effort to be necessary
and relevant. For that reason, the theory of publics shows why blind pursuit of a
maximum number of people in a general public—as opposed to specific publics
as defined by the theory—fails. The situational theory of publics, therefore, leads
communication practitioners to distinguish active segments of a population (i.e.,
active or aware publics) from less active ones (i.e., latent publics or nonpublics) to
avoid cost-ineffective communication (e.g., mass-oriented campaigns; Grunig, 1989;
Grunig & Hunt, 1984). Furthermore, it has provided a critical means of building a
body of knowledge relevant to the strategic management of public relations, such as
how to identify and interact with strategic publics in and around an organization
(e.g., Grunig, Grunig, & Dozier, 2002).
However, a good theory never stagnates (Kruglanski, 2006); the situational
theory has continuously evolved to increase its theoretical power and practical utility
(Grunig, 1997). In this article, we identify several areas that were not addressed
in the original situational theory and propose a more general theory. First, the
theory of publics adopted a narrow conceptualization of active communication
behaviors. It used only information acquisition (information seeking or attending) to
describe an active public. However, if we observe an actively communicating public,
we soon realize that its members engage not only in active information seeking
but also in active information sharing and selecting. In problem solving, selecting
certain information over other information and sharing it with others facilitates
problem solving because such information behaviors can reproduce similar problem
perceptions among people and better mobilize necessary attention to and resources
for dealing with the problem (Chwe, 2001; Gamson, 1992). Second, the concept
of a referent criterion was included in early versions of the situational theory and
then dropped because it failed to predict information seeking and attending (Grunig,
1997). However, several researchers have called attention to the conceptual and
practical benefits of the referent criterion in explaining and classifying the behaviors
of publics (Kim, Ni, & Sha, 2008; Sriramesh, Moghan, & Wei, 2007). We also consider
the utility of the concept and redefine and reinstate it into the situational theory. In
doing so, we test the enhanced predictive power of the referent criterion and discuss
the conceptual utility of the variable.
Third, in its original formulation, the theory of publics only considered perceptual
variables as causal antecedents to communication behavior. Although preserving this
theoretical parsimony is beneficial, it is interesting to test whether there is a more
immediate antecedent variable to communicative action. One promising candidate
variable is a motivational variable (Kruglanski, 1996). Social psychological theorists
122 Journal of Communication 61 (2011) 120–149 © 2011 International Communication Association