Thư viện tri thức trực tuyến
Kho tài liệu với 50,000+ tài liệu học thuật
© 2023 Siêu thị PDF - Kho tài liệu học thuật hàng đầu Việt Nam

Partisan Self-Stereotyping
Nội dung xem thử
Mô tả chi tiết
International Journal of Communication 11(2017), 603–625 1932–8036/20170005
Copyright © 2017 (Jiyoung Han and Daniel B. Wackman). Licensed under the Creative Commons
Attribution Non-commercial No Derivatives (by-nc-nd). Available at http://ijoc.org.
Partisan Self-Stereotyping:
Testing the Salience Hypothesis in a Prediction of Political
Polarization
JIYOUNG HAN1
DANIEL B. WACKMAN
University of Minnesota–Twin Cities, USA
The goal of this study is to theorize the relationship between the news and political
polarization through a lens of group dynamics. Consistent with the salience hypothesis of
the category fit and category accessibility interaction, we first articulate when and how
news exposure makes news consumers think of themselves as Democrats or
Republicans instead of unique individuals. Drawing on group polarization literature, we
further hypothesize partisan self-stereotyping—an automatic reaction to partisan identity
salience—as a mechanism behind the polarizing effect of partisan conflict-framed news.
Two experimental studies provide a consistent pattern of support for our hypotheses.
The implications of these findings were discussed in comparison with extant studies
testing similar news effects under a different theoretical framework—namely, motivated
reasoning.
Keywords: self-stereotyping, polarization, identity salience, conflict framing, selfcategorization
In the presence of conflict, people favor members of their own group (in-group) over members of
the opposing group (out-group; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). In-group favoritism of this sort trumps personal
goals to be fair or maximize the common good (Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971). In addition,
decisions made as group members are known to be more extreme than decisions made as unique
individuals (Abrams & Hogg, 1990; Mackie, 1986; Mackie & Cooper, 1984; Price, 1989). The more intense
intergroup competition grows, the more prominent such group influences become (Turner, Hogg, Oakes,
Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987).
Jiyoung Han: [email protected]
Daniel B. Wackman: [email protected]
Date submitted: 2016–03–02
1 This article has been intensively revised based on Jiyoung Han’s master’s thesis, submitted to the
University of Minnesota–Twin Cities in 2012. This research was supported by a research grant from the
Center for Study of Political Psychology and the KRISS research fund from the School of Journalism and
Mass Communication at the University of Minnesota–Twin Cities. We thank two anonymous reviewers for
their helpful comments and suggestions.
604 Jiyoung Han and Daniel B. Wackman International Journal of Communication 11(2017)
As one of commonly agreed news values in the practice of American journalism (de Vreese,
2012; Neuman, Just, & Crigler, 1992; Price & Tewksbury, 1997), conflict prevails in news coverage of
political events (Cappella & Jamieson, 1997; Lawrence, 2000; Patterson, 1993). As vicarious experience,
partisan conflict-framed news may encourage news consumers to respond to a disputed issue in the news
as the members of the Democratic or Republican Party and show more extreme positions in the direction
of their party. To date, however, very few studies have linked media’s emphasis on partisan conflict to
political polarization in American politics through a lens of group dynamics.
Outside of the political realm, Price (1989) showed the potency of social identity elicited by news
exposure. In his experiment, college participants reading a news story about conflict of opinion between
humanities and hard sciences majors expressed stronger support or opposition in the direction of their
group. No such polarization was observed among participants reading a news story without group conflict.
Drawing on group polarization literature (e.g., Turner et al., 1987), Price argued that polarization between the
two majors should be attributed to the heightened salience of group identity resulted from news exposure.
Along similar lines, many studies acknowledge the role of partisan identity salience in political
polarization (e.g., Druckman, Peterson, & Slothuus, 2013; Iyengar, Sood, & Lelkes, 2012; Levendusky,
2013). However, the nature of identity salience has been poorly discussed in these studies. In particular, it
has been unclear whether media exposure equally makes partisan identity salient to all news consumers
or does so at varying degrees. Without deeper discussion, current literature typically assumes that the
former would be the case and argues that defensive information processing in favor of their political
values is a core factor toward polarization in the American mass public (e.g., G. L. Cohen, 2003;
Druckman et al., 2013; Levendusky, 2013). This line of reasoning, however, warrants further scrutiny
because theories of social identity, on which the aforementioned studies build, argue for the latter: There
are gradations in group identity salience (e.g., Hogg, 2003; Oakes, 1987, 2002; Oakes, Turner, & Haslam,
1991; Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994).
To overcome this limitation, in this study we define partisan identity salience consistent with the
literature of social identity. We then test how variability in partisan identity salience is associated with
attitude polarization along party lines. Given the importance of group identity salience in the development
of social identity theories (Oakes, 1987, 2002), putting a special emphasis on this topic would be a
pertinent way to incorporate group dynamics into the studies of mass media effects.
The Salience Hypothesis: Fit × Accessibility Interaction
Inspired by Bruner’s (1957) idea of categorization process, social identity theorists conceptualize
group identity salience as an interaction between a situation and a perceiver (Hogg, 2003; Hogg & Turner,
1987; Oakes, 1987; Oakes et al., 1991; Turner et al., 1994). For example, when an African American
boards an elevator filled with White Americans, this comparative situation elicits the person’s racial
identity. By contrast, if the elevator were filled with African Americans, no salience of racial identity would
occur in the mind of the person. This example exhibits that group identity salience is emergent and
context specific.